PSYCH 403
ADVANCED PERCEPTION
Introduction to Vision Science
Coren, S., Ward, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Sensation and perception (6th ed.). Wiley.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman and Company
Stillings, N. A., Feinstein, M. H., Garfield, J. L., Rissland, E. L., Rosenbaum, B., Weisler, S. E., & Baker-Ward, L. (1987). Cognitive science: An introduction. The MIT Press.
Haugeland, J. (1986). How can a symbol “mean” something? In Z. W. Pylyshyn and W. Demopoulos (Eds.), Meaning and cognitive structure: Issues in the computational theory of mind (pp. 86-92). Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1980). Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(1), 111-132. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00002053
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition: Towards a foundation for cognitive science. The MIT Press.
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. Hutchinson & Company.
Cummins, R. (1983). The nature of psychological explanation. The MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1989). Computing in cognitive science. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 51-91). The MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 341-423. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99002022
Enns, J. T. (2004). The thinking eye, the seeing brain: Explorations in visual cognition. W. W. Norton and Company.
Fraser, J. (1908). A new visual illusion of direction. British Journal of Psychology, 2(3), 307-320. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1908.tb00182.x
Dawson, M. R. W. (2002). Cognitive science at the University of Alberta. http://www.bcp.psych.ualberta.ca/~mike/Pearl_Street/
Theoretical Approaches to Perception
Treisman, A. (1986, November). Features and objects in visual processing. Scientific American, 255(5), 114B-125. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1186-114b
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman and Company
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.
Titchener, E. B. (1898). The postulates of a structural psychology. Philosophical Review, 7(5), 449-465. https://doi.org/10.2307/2177110
Titchener, E. B. (1901). Experimental psychology: A manual for laboratory practice. The Macmillan Company.
American Psychological Association. (2015). Gestalt. In APA dictionary of psychology (2nd ed., p. 459).
Köhler, W. (1929/1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. Liveright Publishing Corp.
Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91(4), 417-447. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.91.4.417
Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. H. (1959). What the frog’s eye tells the frog’s brain. Proceedings of the IRE, 47 (11), 1940-1951. https://doi.org/10.1109/jrproc.1959.287207
Gregory, R. (1970). The intelligent eye. McGraw Hill.
Attneave, F., & Block, G. (1973). Apparent movement in tridimensional space. Perception & Psychophysics, 13(2), 301-307. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03214143
Gibson, J. J. (1959). Perception as a function of stimulation. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science, Vol. I: Sensory, perceptual, and physiological formulations (pp. 456-501). McGraw-Hill.
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin.
Lee, D. N., & Reddish, P. E. (1981). Plummeting gannets: Paradigm of ecological optics. Nature, 293(5830), 293-294. https://doi.org/10.1038/293293a0
Richards, W. (Ed.). (1988). Natural computation. The MIT Press.
Ullman, S. (1979). The interpretation of visual motion. The MIT Press.
Neurophysiology of Vision
Frisby, J. P. (1979). Seeing: Illusion, brain and mind. Oxford University Press.
Matlin, M. W., & Foley, H. J. (1997). Sensation and perception (4th ed.). Allyn and Bacon.
Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. H. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240(4853), 740-749. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3283936
Cavanagh, P., Tyler, C. W., & Favreau, O. E. (1984). Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 1(8), 893-899. https://doi.org/10.1364/josaa.1.000893
Campbell, F. W., & Maffei, L. (1981). The influence of spatial frequency and contrast on the perception of moving patterns. Vision Research, 21(5), 713-721. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(81)90080-8
Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. H. (1987). Connections between layer 4B of area 17 and thick cytochrome oxidase stripes of area 18 in the squirrel monkey. The Journal of Neuroscience, 7(11), 3371-3377. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.07-11-03371.1987
Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Johann Ambrosius Barth Verlag. Translated by Laurence J. Garey (1994), Brodmann’s Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex. Smith-Gordon.
Wong-Riley, M. (1979). Changes in the visual system of monocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. Brain Research, 171(1), 11-28. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(79)90728-5
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160(1), 106-154. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837
Bonhoeffer, T., & Grinvald, A. (1991). Iso-orientation domains in cat visual cortex are arranged in pinwheellike patterns. Nature, 353(6343), 429-431. https://doi.org/10.1038/353429a0
Obermayer, K., & Blasdel, G. G. (1993). Geometry of orientation and ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 13(10), 4114-4129. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.13-10-04114.1993
Bálint, R. (1909). Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxia, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. Monatsschrift für Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 25(1) 67-81. https://doi.org/10.1159/000210465
Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F. X., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5(6), 679-685. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0960-9822(95)00133-3
Goodale, M., & Milner, D. (2004). Sight unseen: An exploration of conscious and unconscious vision. Oxford University Press.
The Functional Architecture of Vision
Shapley, R., & Lennie, P. (1985). Spatial frequency analysis in the visual system. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 8(1), 547-583. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.002555
Blakemore, C., & Sutton, P. (1969). Size adaptation: A new aftereffect. Science, 166(3902), 245-247. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.166.3902.245
Campbell, F. W., & Robson, J. G. (1968). Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings. Journal of Physiology, 197(3), 551-566. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008574
De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K. (1990). Spatial vision. Oxford University Press.
Maffei, L., & Fiorentini, A. (1973). The visual cortex as a spatial frequency analyser. Vision Research, 13(7), 1255-1267. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(73)90201-0
Gibson, J. J., & Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20(5), 453-467. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0059826
Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(5), 872-876. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040546
Blakemore, C., & Cooper, G. F. (1970). Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature, 228(5270), 477-478. https://doi.org/10.1038/228477a0
Wiesel, T. (1982). Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. Nature, 299(5884), 583-591. https://doi.org/10.1038/299583a0
LeVay, S., Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1980). The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 191(1), 1-51. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.901910102
Katz, L. C. (1999). What’s critical for the critical period in visual cortex? Cell, 99(7), 673-676. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81665-7
Hooks, B. M., & Chen, C. (2007). Critical periods in the visual system: Changing views for a model of experience-dependent plasticity. Neuron, 56(2), 312-326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.003
Sacks, O. (1995). An anthropologist on Mars: Seven paradoxical tales. Knopf.
Brightness, Lightness, & Edge Detection
Burzlaff, W. (1931). Methodologische Beiträge zum Problem der Farbenkonstanz [Methodological notes on the problem of color constancy]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 119, 177-235.
Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception. The MIT Press.
Land, E. H., & McCann, J. J. (1971). Lightness and retinex theory. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 61(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.61.000001
Chevreul, M. E. (1861). The laws of contrast of colour: And their application to the arts of painting, decoration of buildings, mosaic work, tapestry and carpet weaving, calico printing, dress, paper staining, printing, military clothing, illumination, landscape, and flower gardening, &c. Routledge, Warne, and Routledge. English translation by John Spanton.
Craik, K. J. W. (1966). The nature of psychology: A selection of papers, essays and other writings. Cambridge University Press.
O’Brien, V. (1958). Contour perception, illusion and reality. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 48(2), 112-119. https://doi.org/10.1364/josa.48.000112
Cornsweet, T. N. (1970). Visual perception. Academic Press.
Davey, M. P., Maddess, T., & Srinivasan, M. V. (1998). The spatiotemporal properties of the Craik-O’Brien-Cornsweet effect are consistent with ‘filling-in’. Vision Research, 38(13), 2037-2046. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00329-5
Benary, W. (1924). Beobachtungen zu einem Experiment über Helligkeitskontrast. Psychologische Forschung, 5(1), 131-142. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00402398
White, M. (1979). A new effect of pattern on perceived lightness. Perception, 8(4), 413-416. https://doi.org/10.1068/p080413
Gilchrist, A., Kossyfidis, C., Bonato, F., Agostini, T., Cataliotti, J., Li, X., Spehar, B., Annan, V., & Economou, E. (1999). An anchoring theory of lightness perception. Psychological Review, 106(4), 795-834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.795
Adelson, E. H. (2000). Lightness perception and lightness illusions. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed, pp. 339-351). The MIT Press.
Knill, D. C., & Kersten, D. (1991). Apparent surface curvature affects lightness perception. Nature, 351(6323), 228-230. https://doi.org/10.1038/351228a0
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace and Company.
Marr, D. (1976). Early processing of visual information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 275(942), 483-524. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1976.0090
Marr, D., & Hildreth, E. (1980). Theory of edge detection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 207(1167), 187-217. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1980.0020
Schiller, P. (1982). Central connections of the retinal ON and OFF pathways. Nature, 297(5867), 580-583. https://doi.org/10.1038/297580a0
Pearson, D. E., & Robinson, J. A. (1985). Visual communication at very low data rates. Proceedings of the IEEE, 73(4), 795-812. https://doi.org/10.1109/proc.1985.13202
Perceptual Organization
von Ehrenfels, C. (1890). Über Gestaltqualitäten. Vierteljahrsschrift für wissenschaftliche Philosophie, 14(3), 249-292.
Wertheimer, M. (1912/2012). Experimental studies on seeing motion. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9222.003.0003
Köhler, W. (1920). Physical Gestalten. Reprinted in W. D. Ellis (Ed.) (1938), A source book of Gestalt psychology (pp. 17-54). Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Company. https://doi.org/10.1037/11496-003
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace and Company.
Necker, L. A. (1832). Observations on some remarkable optical phænomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phænomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid. The London, Edinburgh, and Dublin Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science, 1(5), 329-337. https://doi.org/10.1080/14786443208647909
Helson, H. (1933). The fundamental propositions of Gestalt psychology. Psychological Review, 40(1), 13-32. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074375
Boring, E. G. (1942). Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century.
Palmer, S. (1992). Common region: A new principle of perceptual grouping. Cognitive Psychology, 24(3) 436-447. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(92)90014-s
Palmer, S. & Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual organization: The role of uniform connectedness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(1) 29-35. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03200760
Hochberg, J., & Brooks, V. (1960). The psychophysics of form: Reversible-perspective drawings of spatial objects. American Journal of Psychology, 73(3), 337-354. https://doi.org/10.2307/1420172
Weisstein, N., & Wong, E. (1986). Figure-ground organization and the spatial and temporal responses of the visual system. In E. C. Schwab & H. C. Nusbaum (Eds.), Pattern recognition by humans and machines, Volume 2: Visual perception (pp. 31-64). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-631402-1.50007-7
Guzmán, A. (1968). Decomposition of a visual scene into three-dimensional bodies. AFIPS '68 (Fall, part I): Proceedings of the December 9-11, 1968, fall joint computer conference, part I (pp.291-304). https://doi.org/10.1145/1476589.1476631
Clowes, M. B. (1971). On seeing things. Artificial Intelligence, 2(1), 79-116. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(71)90005-1
Waltz, D. L. (1972). Generating semantic descriptions of scenes with shadows. Technical Report MAC AI-TR-271. MIT.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman and Company.
Depth Perception
Panum, P. L. (1858). Physiologische untersuchungen über das sehen mit zwei augen. Schers.
Kalloniatis, M., & Luu., C. (2007). Perception of depth. Webvision. https://webvision.med.utah.edu/book/part-viii-psychophysics-of-vision/perception-of-depth/
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). Stereoscopic vision in macaque monkey: Cells sensitive to binocular depth in area 18 of the macaque monkey cortex. Nature, 225(5227), 41-42. https://doi.org/10.1038/225041a0
Poggio, G. F., & Poggio, T. (1984). The analysis of stereopsis. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 7(1) 379-412. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.07.030184.002115
von Helmholtz, H. (1909). Treatise on physiological optics [Translated by J. P. C. Southall). Dover.
Julesz, B. (1964). Binocular depth perception without familiarity cues. Science, 145(3630), 356-362. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.145.3630.356
Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of cyclopean perception. The University of Chicago Press.
Marr, D., & Poggio, T. (1976). Cooperative computation of stereo disparity. Science, 194(4262), 283-287. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.968482
Marr, D., & Poggio, T. (1979). A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 204(1156), 301-328. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1979.0029
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman and Company.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1989). Computing in cognitive science. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 51-91). The MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 341-423. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99002022
Gregory, R. L. (1963). Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling. Nature, 199(4894), 678-691. https://doi.org/10.1038/199678a0
Ponzo, M. (1912). Rapports entre quelques illusions visuelles de contraste angulaire et l’appréciation de grandeur des astres à l’horizon. [Relationship between some visual illusions of angular contrast and the appreciation of the size of the stars on the horizon]. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 58, 327-329.
Müller-Lyer, F. C. (1889). Optische Urteilstauschungen. Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie, Physiologische Abteilung, 2, 263-270.
Gregory, R. L. (1968, November). Visual illusions. Scientific American, 219(5), 66-76. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1168-66
Gregory, R. L. (1990). Eye and brain: The psychology of seeing (4th ed.). Oxford University Press.
Gandhi, T., Kalia, A., Ganesh, S., & Sinha, P. (2015). Immediate susceptibility to visual illusions after sight onset. Current Biology, 25(9), R358-R359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.005
McGraw, K. O. (2003). Image size enhancement theory. http://www.olemiss.edu/courses/psy214/Readings/Illusions/Image%20Size%20Enhancement%20Theory.pdf
Schwarzkopf, D. S., Song, C., & Rees, G. (2011). The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nature Neuroscience, 14(1), 28-30. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2706
Time Perception
Schiffman, H. R. (2000). Sensation and perception (5th ed.). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Oxford University Press. (2006). Concise Oxford English dictionary (11th ed.). Oxford University Press
Pöppel, E. (1978). Time perception. In R. Held, H. W. Leibowitz, & H.-L. Teuber (Eds.), Handbook of sensory physiology, vol. VIII: Perception (pp. 713-729). Springer-Verlag.
Coren, S., Ward, L. M., & Enns, J. T. (1999). Sensation and perception (5th ed.). Harcourt Brace College Publishers.
Le Poidevin, R. (2019). The experience and perception of time. Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/time-experience/
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Henry Holt and Company.
de Mairan, J. J. d’O. (1733). Traité physique et historique de l’aurore boréale. L’Imprimerie Royale.
Kleitman, N. (1939). Sleep and wakefulness. University of Chicago Press.
Czeisler, C. A., Duffy, J. F., Shanahan, T. L., Brown, E. N., Mitchell, J. F., Rimmer, D. W., Ronda, J. M., Silva, E. J., Allan, J. S., Emens, J. S., Dijk, D. J., & Kronauer, R. E. (1999). Stability, precision, and near 24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science, 284(5423), 2177-2181. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.284.5423.2177
Hattar, S., Liao, H. W., Takao, M., Berson, D. M., & Yau K.-W. (2002). Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: Architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science, 295(5557), 1065-1070. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1069609
Hoagland, H. (1933). The physiological control of judgment of duration: evidence for a chemical clock. Journal of General Psychology, 9(2), 267-287. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1933.9920937
Hoagland, H. (1935). Pacemakers in relation to aspects of behavior. Macmillan.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Time-estimation at reduced body temperature. American Journal of Psychology, 79(3), 475-479. https://doi.org/10.2307/1420890
Rao, S., Mayer, A., & Harrington, D. (2001). The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nature Neuroscience, 4(3), 317-323. https://doi.org/10.1038/85191
Mach, E. (1865). Untersuchungen über den Zeitsinn des Ohres. Sitzungsberichte der mathematisch-naturwissenschaftlichen Classe der Kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften, 51, II. Abteilung, 133-150.
White, C. T.(1963). Temporal numerosity and the psychological unit of duration. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(12), 1-37. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093860
Efron, R. (1967). The duration of the present. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 138(2), 713-729. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1967.tb55017.x
von Vierordt, K. (1868). Der Zeitsinn nach Versuchen. Laupp.
Wearden, J. H. (2005). The wrong tree: Time perception and time experience in the elderly. In J. Duncan, L. Phillips, & P. McLeod (Eds.), Measuring the mind: Speed, age, and control (pp. 137-158). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198566427.003.0006
Sackett, A. M., Meyvis, T., Nelson, L. D., Converse, B. A., & Sackett, A. L. (2010). You’re having fun when time flies: The hedonic consequences of subjective time progression. Psychological Science, 21(1), 111-117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609354832
Stetson, C., Fiesta, M. P., & Eagleman, D. M. (2007). Does time really slow down during a frightening event? PLoS ONE, 2(12), e1295. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001295
Ornstein, R. E. (1969). On the experience of time. Penguin Books.
Mulligan, R. M., & Schiffman, H. R. (1979). Temporal experience as a function of organization in memory. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 14(6), 417-420. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03329496
Thomas, E. A. C., & Weaver, W. B. (1975). Cognitive processing and time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 17(4), 363-367. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03199347
Underwood, G. (1975). Attention and perception of duration during encoding and retrieval. Perception, 4(3), 291-296. https://doi.org/10.1068/p040291
Zakay, D. (1989). Subjective time and attentional resource allocation: An integrated model of time estimation. In I. Levin & D. Zakay (Eds.), Advances in psychology, 59. Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective (pp. 365-397). North-Holland. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61047-X
Chaston, A., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Time estimation: The effect of cortically mediated attention. Brain and Cognition, 55(2), 286-289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.013
Allen, L. (2006). Science confirms the obvious! Popular Science. https://www.popsci.com/scitech/article/2006-05/science-confirms-obvious/
Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A. & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(6), 972-982. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03205512
Frassinetti, F., Magnani, B., & Oliveri, M. (2009). Prismatic lenses shift time perception. Psychological Science, 20(8), 949-954. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x
Cooperrider, K., & Núñez, R. (2016, November). How we make sense of time. Scientific American Mind, 27(6), 38-43. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamericanmind1116-38
Joubert, C. E. (1990). Subjective expectations of the acceleration of time with again. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70(1), 334. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1990.70.1.334
Object Perception
Biederman, I. (1981). On the semantics of a glance at a scene. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (pp. 213-253). Lawrence Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315512372-8
Selfridge, O. G. (1959). Pandemonium: A paradigm for learning. In D. V. Blake & A. M. Uttley (Eds.)Proceedings of the symposium on the mechanisation of thought processes (pp. 511-529). Her Majesty's Stationery Office.
Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1972). Human information processing: An introduction to psychology. Academic Press.
Marr, D., & Nishihara, H. K. (1978). Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three dimensional structure. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 200(1140), 269-294. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1978.0020
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94(2), 115-147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.94.2.115
Biederman, I. (1995). Visual object recognition. In S. F. Kosslyn & D. N. Osherson (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science, Vol. 2. Visual Cognition (2nd ed., pp. 121-165). The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3965.003.0007
Thrun, S., Montemerlo, M., Dahlkamp, H., Stavens, D., Aron, A., Diebel, J. Fong, P., Gale, J., Halpenny, M., Hoffmann, G., Lau, K., Oakley, C., Palatucci, M., Pratt, V., Stang, P., Strohband, S., Dupont, C., Jendrossek, L. E., Koelen, C., ... Mahoney, P. (2006). Stanley: The robot that won the DARPA Grand Challenge. Journal of Field Robotics, 23(9), 661-692. https://doi.org/10.1002/rob.20147
Schoettle, B. (2017). Sensor fusion: A comparison of sensing capabilities of human drivers and highly automated vehicles (SWT-2017-12). Sustainable Worldwide Transportation, The University of Michigan. http://www.umich.edu/~umtriswt/PDF/SWT-2017-12.pdf
Face Perception
Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. N. (1998). What is “special” about face perception? Psychological Review, 105(3), 482-498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.105.3.482
Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 361(1476), 2109-2128. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1934
Tanaka, J. N. & Farah, M. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 46A(2), 225-245. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749308401045
Thompson, P. (1980). Margaret Thatcher: A new illusion. Perception, 9(4), 483-484. https://doi.org/10.1068/p090483
Allison, T., Ginter, H., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A. C., Puce, A., Luby, M., & Spencer, D. D. (1994). Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 71(2), 821-825. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1994.71.2.821
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302-4311. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.17-11-04302.1997
Kuskowski, M. A., & Pardo, J. V. (1999). The role of the fusiform gyrus in successful encoding of face stimuli. NeuroImage, 9(6), 599-610. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1999.0442
McNeil, J. E., & Warrington, E. K. (1993). Prosopagnosia: A face-specific disorder. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 46(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640749308401064
Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., & Behrmann, M. (1997). What is special about face recognition? Nineteen experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 555-604. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.555
Gauthier, I., Williams, P., Tarr, M. J., & Tanaka, J. (1998). Training ‘greeble’ experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Research, 38(15-16), 2401-2428. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00442-2
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1977). Developmental changes in the representation of faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23(1), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(77)90069-8
Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(2), 107-117. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.2.107
Puce, A., Allison, T., Asgari, M., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(16), 5205-5215. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.16-16-05205.1996
Arcurio, L. R., Gold, J. M., & James, T. M. (2012). The response of face-selective cortex with single face parts and part combinations. Neuropsychologia, 50(10), 2454-2459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.016
Liu, J., Harris, A., & Kanwisher, N. (2010). Perception of face parts and face configurations: An fMRI Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 203-211. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21203
Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Anderson, A. W., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J. C. (1999). Activation of the middle fusiform ‘face area’ increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(6), 568-573. https://doi.org/10.1038/9224
Gauthier, I., Behrmann, M., & Tarr, M. J. (1999). Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(4), 349-370. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563472
De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., Grossi, D., & Nichelli, P. (1991). Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex, 27(2), 213-221. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80125-6
Gainotti, G., & Marra, C. (2011). Differential contribution of right and left temporo-occipital and anterior temporal lesions to face recognition disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, Article 55. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00055
Michelon, P., & Biederman, I. (2003). Less impairment in face imagery than face perception in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 41(4), 421-441. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00176-8
Hecaen, H., & Angelergues, R. (1962). Agnosia for faces (prosopagnosia). Archives of Neurology, 7(2), 24-32. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1962.04210020014002
Bodamer, A. (1947). Die Prosop-Agnosie. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 179(1-2), 6-53. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00352849
Bruyer, R., Laterre, C., Seron, X., Feyereisen, P., Srypstein, E., Pierrard, E., & Rectern, D. (1983). A case of prosopagnosia with some preserved covert remembrance of familiar faces. Brain and Cognition, 2>(3), 257-284. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(83)90014-3
Capgras, J. M. J., & Reboul-Lachaux, J. (1923). L’illusion des ‘sosies’ dans un délire systématisé chronique. Bulletin de la Société clinique de médecine mentale, 11, 6-16. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957154x9400501709
Hirstein, W., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1997). Capgras syndrome: A novel probe for understanding the neural representation and familiarity of persons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 264(1380), 437-444. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1997.0062
Ellis, H. D., & Lewis, M. B. (2001). Capgras delusion: A window on face recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(4), 149-156. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01620-x
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 223-233. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01482-0
Bauer, R. M. (1984). Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test. Neuropsychologia, 22(4), 457-469. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(84)90040-x
De Haan, E. H. F., Bauer, R. M., & Greve, K. W. (1992). Behavioural and physiological evidence for covert recognition in a prosopagnosic patient. Cortex, 28(1), 77-95. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80167-0
Duchaine, B. C., Parker, H., & Nakayama, K. (2003). Normal recognition of emotion in a prosopagnosic. Perception, 32(7), 827-838. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5067
Tranel, D., Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1988). Intact recognition of facial expression, gender, and age in patients with impaired recognition of face identity. Neurology, 38(5), 690-696. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.38.5.690
Courbon, P., & Fail, G. (1927). Syndrome d’illusion de Frégoli et schizophrénie. Bulletin de la Societé Clinique et Médecin Mentale, 15, 121-125. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957154x9400501710
Hudson, A. J., & Grace, G. M. (2000). Misidentification syndromes related to face specific area in the fusiform gyrus. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 69(5), 645-648. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.69.5.645
Ramachandran, V. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain. Harper Perennial.
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. Macmillan.
Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science, 1(2), 115-121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00079.x
Pallett, P. M., Link, S., & Lee, K. (2010). New “golden” ratios for facial beauty. Vision Research, 50(2), 149-154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.003
Perrett, D. I., May, K. A., & Yoshikawa, S. (1994). Facial shape and judgements of female attractiveness. Nature, 368(6468), 239-242. https://doi.org/10.1038/368239a0
Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., & Comer, R. (1995). Human female orgasm and mate fluctuating asymmetry. Animal Behaviour, 50(6), 1601- 1615. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(95)80014-x
Little, A. C., & Jones, B. (2003). Evidence against perceptual bias views for symmetry preferences in human faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 270(1526), 1759-1763. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2445
Hamermesh, D. S., & Abrevaya, J. (2013). Beauty is the promise of happiness? European Economic Review, 64, 351-368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2013.09.005
Mocan, N., & Tekin, E. (2010). Ugly criminals. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 92(1), 15-30. https://doi.org/10.1162/rest.2009.11757
Morin, R. (2006, February 17). The ugly face of crime. Washington Post. https://www.washingtonpost.com/archive/politics/2006/02/17/the-ugly-face-of-crime/
Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science, 308(5728), 1623-1626. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1110589
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592-598. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x
Montepare, J. M., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (1998). Person perception comes of age: The salience of age in social judgments. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 93-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60383-4
Antonakis, J., & Dalgas, O. (2009). Predicting elections: Child’s play! Science, 323(5918), 1183. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1167748
Cross, J. F., Cross, J., & Daly, J. (1971). Sex, race, age, and beauty as factors in recognition of faces. Perception & Psychophysics, 10(6), 393-396. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03210319
Bower, G. H., & Karlin, M. H. (1974). Depth of processing pictures of faces and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103(4), 751-757. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037190
Epley, N., & Whitchurch, E. (2008). Mirror, mirror on the wall: Enhancement in self-recognition. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(9), 1159-1170. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208318601
Synesthesia
van Campen, C. (2008). The hidden sense: Synesthesia in art and science. The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/7492.001.0001
Fechner, G. T. (1871). Vorschule der Aesthetik. Breitkopf und Hartel. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139854580
Galton, F. (1880a). Visualized numerals. Nature, 21(533), 252-256. https://doi.org/10.1038/021252a0
Galton, F. (1880b). Visualized numerals. Nature, 21(536), 252-256. https://doi.org/10.1038/021323a0
Galton, F. (1880c). Visualized numerals. Nature, 21(543), 494-495. https://doi.org/10.1038/021252a0
Galton, F. (1881). The visions of sane persons. Fortnightly Review, 29, 729-740. http://galton.org/essays/1880-1889/galton-1881-fort-rev-visions-sane-persons.pdf
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. Macmillan.
Calkins, M. W. (1894). Synæsthesia. American Journal of Psychology, 7(10, 90-107. https://doi.org/10.2307/1412040
Baron-Cohen, S., Wyke, M. A., & Binnie, C. (1987). Hearing words and seeing colours: An experimental investigation of a case of synaesthesia. Perception, 16(6), 761-767. https://doi.org/10.1068/p160761
Baron-Cohen, S,, Burt, L., Smith-Laittan, F., Harrison, J., & Bolton, P. (1996). Synaesthesia: Prevalence and familiality. Perception, 25(9), 1073-1079. https://doi.org/10.1068/p251073
Asher, J., Aitken, M. R. F., Farooqi, N., Kurmani, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Diagnosing and phenotyping visual synaesthesia: A preliminary evaluation of the Revised Test of Genuineness (TOG-R). Cortex, 42(2), 137-146. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70337-x
Cytowic, R. E. (2004). Synesthesia: anomalous binding of qualia and categories. In G. Adelman & B. H. Smith (Eds.), Encyclopedia of neuroscience (3rd ed., pp. 7-26). Elsevier.
Eagleman, D. M., Kagana, A. D. Nelson, S. S., Sagaram, D. & Sarma, A. K. (2007). A standardized test battery for the study of synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 159(1), 139-145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.07.012
Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E., Witherby, S. A., Fraser, C., Scott, K., & Ward, J. (2006). Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences. Perception, 35(8), 1024-1033. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5469
Simner, J., & Carmichael, D. A. (2015). Is synaesthesia a dominantly female trait? Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(2-3), 68-76. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2015.1019441
Simner, J., Ward, J., Lanz, M., Jansari, A., Noonan, K., Glover, L., & Oakley, D. A. (2005). Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(8), 1069-1085. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290500200122
Hubbard, E. M. (2007). Neurophysiology of synesthesia. Current Psychiatry Reports, 9(3), 193-199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-007-0018-6
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001a). Synaesthesia -- A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3-34. https://www.ingentaconnect.com/contentone/imp/jcs/2001/00000008/00000012/1244
Hubbard, E. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2005). Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia. Neuron, 48(3), 509-520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.012
Hubbard, E. M., Arman, A. C., Ramachandran, V. S., & Boynton, G. M. (2005). Individual differences among grapheme-color synesthetes: Brain-behavior correlations. Neuron, 45(6), 975-985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.008
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001b). Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 268(1470), 979-983. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1576
Hubbard, E. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2001). Cross wiring and the neural basis of synaesthesia. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 42(4), S712.
Nunn, J. A., Gregory, L. J., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C. R., Parslow, D. M., Morgan, M. J., Morris, R. G., Bullmore, E. T., Baron-Cohen, S., & Gray, J. A. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: Activation of V4/V8 by spoken words. Nature Neuroscience, 5(4), 371-375. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn818
Mills, C. B., Boteler, E. H., & Oliver, G. K. (1999). Digit synaesthesia: A case study using a Stroop-type test. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16(2), 181-191. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432999380951
Johnson, A., Jepma, A., De Jong, R. (2007). Colours sometimes count: Awareness and bidirectionality in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(10), 1406-1422. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210601063597
Ramachandran, V. S. (2000, March 24). Perceptual consequences of cortical cross-wiring: Phantom limb pain and synesthesia [Presentation]. The First International Consensus Meeting on the Management of Phantom Limb Pain, Oxford, UK.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., Duffy, P. L., Zanna, M. P., & Merikle, P. M. (2006). The role of meaning in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Cortex, 42(2), 243-252. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70349-6
Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., Cudahy, C., & Merikle, P. M. (2000). Five plus two equals yellow. Nature, 406(6794), 365. https://doi.org/10.1038/35019148
Ramachandran, V. S., Hubbard, E. M., & Butcher, P. A. (2003). “Higher” and “lower” forms of synesthesia may arise from cross-wiring at different cortical stages. Journal of Vision, 2(7), 265, 265a. https://doi.org/10.1167/2.7.265
Dixon, M. J., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2004). Not all synaesthetes are created equal: Projector versus associator synaesthetes. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(3), 335-343. https://doi.org/10.3758/cabn.4.3.335
Ward, J., Li, R., Salih, S., & Sagiv, N. (2007). Varieties of grapheme-colour synaesthesia: A new theory of phenomenological and behavioural differences. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(4), 913-931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.012
Hubbard, E. M., Manohar, S., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2006). Contrast affects the strength of synesthetic colors. Cortex, 42(2), 184-194. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70343-5
Rouw, R. & Scholte, H. S. (2007). Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia. Nature Neuroscience, 10(6), 792-797. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1906
Grossenbacher, P. G., & Lovelace, C. T. (2001). Mechanisms of synesthesia: Cognitive and physiological constraints. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5(1), 36-41. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01571-0
Cytowic, R. E., & Eagleman, D. M. (2009). Wednesday is indigo blue: Discovering the brain of synesthesia. The MIT Press.
Ward, J., & Simner, J. (2003). Lexical-gustatory synaesthesia: Linguistic and conceptual factors. Cognition, 89(3), 237-261. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00122-7
Witthoft, N., & Winawer, J. (2006). Synesthetic colors determined by having colored refrigerator magnets in childhood. Cortex, 42(2), 175-183. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70342-3
Witthoft, N., & Winawer, J. (2013). Learning, memory, and synesthesia. Psychological Science, 24(3), 258-265. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612452573
Witthoft, N., Winawer, J., & Eagleman, D. M. (2015). Prevalence of learned grapheme-color pairings in a large online sample of synesthetes. PLoS ONE, 10(3), Article e0118996. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118996
Cytowic, R. (2009). When senses intersect. Scientific American. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/when-senses-intersect/
Ward, J. (2008). The frog who croaked blue: Synesthesia and the mixing of the senses. Routledge.
Esterman, M., Verstynen, T., Ivry, R. B., & Robertson, L. C. (2006). Coming unbound: Disrupting automatic integration of synesthetic color and graphemes by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right parietal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(9), 1570-1576. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1570
Ramachandran, V. S. (2004). Purple numbers and sharp cheese. In V. S. Ramachandran, A brief tour of human consciousness: From impostor poodles to purple numbers (pp. 60-82). Pi Press.
Köhler, W. (1929/1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. Liveright Publishing Corp.
Perception and Art
Snow, C. P. (1959/2013). The two cultures and the scientific revolution. Martino Fine Books.
Weiten, W., & McCann, D. (2007). Psychology: Themes and variations (1st Canadian ed.). Thomson Nelson.
Livingstone, M. S. (2002). Vision and art: The biology of seeing. Harry N. Abrams.
Penrose, L. S., & Penrose, R. (1958). Impossible objects: A special type of illusion. British Journal of Psychology, 49(1), 31-33. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1958.tb00634.x
Troncoso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., Otero-Millan, J., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Microsaccades drive illusory motion in the Enigma illusion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(41), 16033-16038. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0709389105
Kitaoka, A., & Ashida, H. (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of the peripheral drift illusion. Vision: The Journal of the Vision Society of Japan, 15(4), 261-262. http://www.visionsociety.jp/vision/koumokuPDF/12kouen/Y2003.15.04.01.pdf
Schyns, P. G., & Oliva, A. (1999). Dr. Angry and Mr. Smile: When categorization flexibly modifies the perception of faces in rapid visual presentations. Cognition, 69(3), 243-265. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00069-9
Harmon, L. D., & Julesz, B. (1973, November). The recognition of faces. Scientific American, 229(5), 71-82. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1173-70
Harmon, L. D., & Julesz, B. (1973). Masking in visual recognition: Effects of two-dimensional filtered noise. Science, 180(4091), 1194-1197. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.180.4091.1194
Freud, S. (1910). Eine Kindheitserinnerung des Leonardo da Vinci [Leonardo da Vinci, a memory of his childhood]. Franz Deuticke.
Livingstone, M. S. (2000). Is it warm? Is it real? Or just low spatial frequency? Science, 290(5495), 1299. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.290.5495.1299b
Kontsevich, L. L., & Tyler, C. W. (2004). What makes Mona Lisa smile? Vision Research, 44(13), 1493-1498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.027
Zeki, S. (1998). Art and the brain. Dædalus, 127, 71-103. https://www.mitpressjournals.org/loi/daed
Zeki, S. (1999). Inner vision: An exploration of art and the brain. Oxford University Press.
Zeki, S. (2001). Artistic creativity and the brain. Science, 293(5527), 51-52. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062331
Solso, R. L. (2000). The cognitive neuroscience of art: A preliminary fMRI observation. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(8-9), 75-85. https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/2000/00000007/f0020008/1043
Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W. (1999). The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(6-7), 15-51. https://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/1999/00000006/f0020006/949
Ramachandran, V. S. (2003). The artful brain. BBC Reith Lectures. https://www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/reith2003/lecture3.shtml
Ramachandran, V. S. (2004). The artful brain. In V. S. Ramachandran, A brief tour of human consciousness: From impostor poodles to purple numbers (pp. 40-59). Pi Press.
Ramachandran, V. S. (2006). Synesthesia and the universal principles of art. The New York Academies of Science. https://web.archive.org/web/20061115061157/http://www.nyas.org/ebriefreps/main.asp?intSubsectionID=3574
Hanson, H. M. (1959). Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(5), 321-334. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0042606
Tinbergen, N. (1957). The herring gull’s world. Oxford University Press.
Fechner, G. T. (1865). Über die Frage des golden Schnitts [On the question of the golden section]. Archiv für die zeichnenden Künste, 11, 100-112.
Fibonacci. (1202). Liber Abaci [The book of calculation].
Gottlieb, E. L., & Marquardt, S. R. (2002). JCO interviews Dr. Stephen R. Marquardt on the Golden Decagon and human facial beauty. Journal of Clinical Orthodontics, 36(6), 339-347. https://www.jco-online.com/archive/2002/06/339-jco-interviews-dr-stephen-r-marquardt-on-the-golden-decagon-and-human-facial-beauty/
Di Dio, C., Macalusa, E., & Rizzolati, G. (2007). The golden beauty: Brain response to Classical and Renaissance sculptures. Public Library of Science One, 2(11), e1201.
Green, C. D. (1995). All that glitters: A review of psychological research on the aesthetics of the Golden Section. Perception, 24(8), 937-968. https://doi.org/10.1068/p240937
Cavanagh, P. (2005). The artist as neuroscientist. Nature, 434(7031), 301-307. https://doi.org/10.1038/434301a
Casati, R., & Cavanagh, P. (2019). The visual world of shadows. The MIT Press.
Bertamini, M., Latto, R., & Spooner, A. (2003). The Venus effect: People's understanding of mirror reflections in paintings. Perception, 32(5), 593-599. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3418
The Psychology of Food and Eating
Logue, A. W. (2015). The psychology of eating and drinking (4th ed.). Brunner-Routledge. https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9780203387924
Capaldi, E. D. (Ed.) (2001). Why we eat what we eat: The psychology of eating. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10291-000
Shepherd, G. M. (2006). Smell images and the flavour system in the human brain. Nature, 444(7117), 316-321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05405
Schachter, S., & Gross, L. P. (1968). Manipulated time and eating behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10(2), 98-106. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026285
Noble, A. C. (1996). Taste-aroma interactions. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 7(12), 439-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-2244(96)10044-3
DuBose, C., Cardello, A. V., & Maller, O. (1980). Effects of colorants and flavorants on identification, perceived flavor intensity, and hedonic quality of fruit-flavored beverages and cake. Journal of Food Science, 45(5), 1393-1399. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2621.1980.tb06562.x
Morrot, G., Brochet, F., & Dubourdieu, D. (2001). The color of odors. Brain and Language, 79(2), 309-320. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2493
Delwiche, J. (2004). The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor. Food Quality and Preference, 15(2), 137-146. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0950-3293(03)00041-7
Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2004). The role of auditory cues in modulating the perceived crispness and staleness of potato chips. Journal of Sensory Studies, 19(5), 347-363. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-459x.2004.080403.x
Green, B. C. (1996). Chemesthesis: Pungency as a component of flavor. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 7(12), 415-420. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-2244(96)10043-1
Tuorila, H. (1996). Hedonic responses to flavor and their implications for food acceptance. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 7(12), 453-456. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-2244(96)10048-0
Gray, K. (2012). The power of good intentions: Perceived benevolence soothes pain, increases pleasure, and improves taste. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 639-645. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1948550611433470
Engen, T. (1982). The perception of odors. Academic Press. https://www.sciencedirect.com/book/9780122393501/the-perception-of-odors
Drewnowski, A. (1997). Taste preferences and food intake. Annual Review of Nutrition, 17(1), 237-253. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.nutr.17.1.237
Sørensen, L. B., Møller, P., Flint, A., Martens, M., & Raben, A. (2003). Effect of sensory perception of foods on appetite and food intake: A review of studies on humans. International Journal of Obesity, 27(10), 1152-1166. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802391
Sullivan, S. A., & Birch, L. L. (1994). Infant dietary experience and acceptance of solid foods. Pediatrics, 93(2), 271-277. https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/93/2/271
Birch, L. L., McPhee, L., Shoba, B. C., Pirok, E., & Steinberg, L. (1987). What kind of exposure reduces children’s food neophobia? Appetite, 9(3), 171-178. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0195-6663(87)80011-9
Escalona, S. K. (1945). Feeding disturbances in very young children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 15(1), 76-80. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1945.tb04918.x
Wansink, B., Painter, J., & van Ittersum, K. (2001, December). Descriptive menu labels’ effect on sales. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 42(6), 68-72. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0010880401426008
Wansink, B. (2004). Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annual Review of Nutrition, 24(1), 455-479. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.nutr.24.012003.132140
Wansink, B. (2006). Mindless eating: Why we eat more than we think. Bantam.
Wansink, B., van Ittersum, K., & Painter, J. E. (2006). Ice cream illusions: Bowl size, spoon size, and self-served portion sizes. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 31(3), 240-243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2006.04.003
Wansink, B., Painter, J. E., & North, J. (2005). Bottomless bowls: Why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obesity Research, 13(1), 93-100. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2005.12
Painter, J. E., Wansink, B., & Heiggelke, J. B. (2002). How visibility and convenience influence candy consumption. Appetite, 38(3), 237-238. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.2002.0485
de Castro, J. M., & Brewer, E. M. (1992). The amount eaten in meals by humans is a power function of the number of people present. Physiology and Behavior, 51(1), 121-125. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(92)90212-k
Robinson, E., Aveyard, P., Daley, A., Jolly, K., Lewis, A., Lycett, D., & Higgs, S. (2013). Eating attentively: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of food intake memory and awareness on eating. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 97(4), 728-742. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.112.045245
Rozin, P., Dow, S., Moscovitch, M., & Rajaram, S. (1998). What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychological Science, 9(5), 392-396. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00073
Wansink, B. & Park, S. B. (2001). At the movies: How external cues and perceived taste impact consumption volume. Food Quality and Preference, 12(1), 69-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0950-3293(00)00031-8i
Turnwald, B. P., Bertoldo, J. D., Perry, M. A., Policastro, P., Timmons, M., Bosso, C., Connors, P., Valgenti, R. T., Pine, L., Challamel, G., Gardner, C. D., & Crum, A. J. (2019). Increasing vegetable intake by emphasizing tasty and enjoyable attributes: A randomized controlled multisite intervention for taste-focused labeling. Psychological Science, 30(11), 1603-1615. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619872191
Wansink, B., Painter, J. E., & Lee, Y. K. (2006). The office candy dish: Proximity’s influence on estimated and actual candy consumption. International Journal of Obesity, 30(5), 871-875. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803217
Kahn, B. E., & Wansink, B. (2004). The influence of assortment structure on perceived variety and consumption quantities. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(4), 519-533. https://doi.org/10.1086/380286
Ghorayshi, A. (2012, June 25). Too big to chug: How our sodas got so huge. Mother Jones. https://www.motherjones.com/media/2012/06/supersize-biggest-sodas-mcdonalds-big-gulp-chart/
Wansink, B., & Kim, J. (2005). Bad popcorn in big buckets: Portion size can influence intake as much as taste. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 37(5), 242-245. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1499-4046(06)60278-9
Chandon, P., & Wansink, B. (2002). When are stockpiled products consumed faster? A convenience-salience framework of post-purchase consumption incidence and quantity. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(3), 321-335. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.39.3.321.19111
Wansink, B., & van Ittersum, K. (2005). Shape of glass and amount of alcohol poured: Comparative study of effect of practice and concentration. British Medical Journal, 331(7531), 1512-1514. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.331.7531.1512
Wansink, B., & van Ittersum, K. (2007). Portion size me: Downsizing our consumption norms. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 107(7), 1103-1106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2007.05.019
van Ittersum, K., & Wansink, B. (2012). Plate size and color suggestibility: The Delboeuf illusion’s bias on serving and eating behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 215-228. https://doi.org/10.1086/662615
Delboeuf, F. J. (1865). Note sur certaines illusions d’optique: Essai d’une théorie psychophysique de la maniere dont l’oeil apprécie les distances et les angles. Bulletins de l’Académie Royale des Sciences, Lettres et Beaux-arts de Belgique, 19, 195-216.
Neuromagic
Binet, A. (1894). Psychology of prestidigitation. Smithsonian Report for 1894 (pp. 555-571). Government Printing Office.
Jastrow, J. (1897). Review of magic stage illusions and scientific diversions, including trick photography. Science, 6(153), 850-851. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6.153.850-b
Jastrow, J. (1888). The psychology of deception. Popular Science Monthly, 34, 145-157. https://en.wikisource.org/wiki/Popular_Science_Monthly/Volume_34/December_1888/The_Psychology_of_Deception
Jastrow, J. (1896). Psychological notes upon sleight-of-hand experts. Science, 3(71), 685-689. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3.71.685
Triplett, N. (1900). The psychology of conjuring deceptions. American Journal of Psychology, 11(4), 439-510. https://doi.org/10.2307/1412365
Tompkins, M. L. (2018, December 19). Science of magic bibliography - 2018 update. https://www.matt-tompkins.com/blog/2018/12/19/science-of-magic-bibliography-2018-update
Martinez-Conde, S., & Macknik, S. L. (2007). Mind tricks. Nature, 448(7152), 414. https://doi.org/10.1038/448414a
Kuhn, G., Amlani, A. A., & Rensink, R. A. (2008). Towards a science of magic. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(9), 349-354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.008
Macknik, S. L., King, M., Randi, J., Robbins, A., Teller, Thompson, J., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Attention and awareness in stage magic: Turning tricks into research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(11), 871-879. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2473
Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S. L., Robbins, A., McCamy, M., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2011). Stronger misdirection in curved than in straight motion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, Article 133. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00133
Martinez-Conde, S. & Macknik, S. L. (2008, December). Magic and the brain. Scientific American, 299(6), 72, 74-79. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1208-72
Kuhn, G., & Tatler, B. W. (2005). Magic and fixation: Now you don’t see it, now you do. Perception, 34(9), 1155-1161. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3409bn1
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (2000). On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions. Visual Cognition, 7(1-3), 127-149. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062800394720
Kuhn, G., Teszka, R., Tenaw, N., & Kingstone, A. (2016). Don’t be fooled! Attentional responses to social cues in a face-to-face and video magic trick reveals greater top-down control for overt than covert attention. Cognition, 146, 136-142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.005
Barnhart, A. S., & Goldinger, S. D. (2014). Blinded by magic: Eye-movements reveal the misdirection of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1461. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01461
Hall, L., Johansson, P., Tärning, B., Sikström, S., & Deutgen, T. (2010). Magic at the marketplace: Choice blindness for the taste of jam and the smell of tea. Cognition, 117(1), 54-61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.010
Parris, B. A., Kuhn, G., Mizon, G. A., Benattayallah, A., & Hodgson, T. L. (2009). Imaging the impossible: An fMRI study of impossible causal relationships in magic tricks. NeuroImage, 45(3), 1033-1039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.036
Olson, J. A., Amlani, A. A., Raz, A., & Rensink, R. A. (2015). Influencing choice without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 225-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.004
Kuhn, G., & Land, M. F. (2006). There’s more to magic than meets the eye. Current Biology, 16(22), 950-951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.012
Rieiro, H., Martinez-Conde, S., & Macknik, S. L. (2013). Perceptual elements in Penn & Teller’s “Cups and Balls” magic trick. PeerJ, 1, Article e19. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.19
Thomas, C., Didierjean, A., Maquestiaux, F., & Gygax, P. (2015). Does magic offer a cryptozoology ground for psychology? Review of General Psychology, 19(2), 117-128. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000041
Lamont, P., Henderson, J. M., & Smith, T. J. (2010). Where science and magic meet: The illusion of a “science of magic”. Review of General Psychology, 14(1), 16-21. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017157
Lamont, P. (2015). Problems with the mapping of magic tricks. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 855. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00855
Kuhn, G., & Martinez, L. M. (2012). Misdirection — Past, present, and the future. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, Article 172. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00172
Rensink, R. A., & Kuhn, G. (2015). The possibility of a science of magic. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1576. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01576